Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Anemia and risk for cognitive decline in chronic kidney disease

Abstract

Background

Anemia is common among patients with chronic kidney disease (CKD) but its health consequences are poorly defined. The aim of this study was to determine the relationship between anemia and cognitive decline in older adults with CKD.

Methods

We studied a subgroup of 762 adults age ≥55 years with CKD participating in the Chronic Renal Insufficiency Cohort (CRIC) study. Anemia was defined according to the World Health Organization criteria (hemoglobin <13 g/dL for men and <12 g/dL for women). Cognitive function was assessed annually with a battery of six tests. We used logistic regression to determine the association between anemia and baseline cognitive impairment on each test, defined as a cognitive score more than one standard deviation from the mean, and mixed effects models to determine the relation between anemia and change in cognitive function during follow-up after adjustment for demographic and clinical characteristics.

Results

Of 762 participants with mean estimated glomerular filtration rate of 42.7 ± 16.4 ml/min/1.73 m2, 349 (46 %) had anemia. Anemia was not independently associated with baseline cognitive impairment on any test after adjustment for demographic and clinical characteristics. Over a median 2.9 (IQR 2.6–3.0) years of follow-up, there was no independent association between anemia and change in cognitive function on any of the six cognitive tests.

Conclusions

Among older adults with CKD, anemia was not independently associated with baseline cognitive function or decline.

Peer Review reports

Background

Cognitive impairment affects up to one-third of patients with advanced chronic kidney disease (CKD) and end-stage renal disease (ESRD) [1, 2]. The prevalence of cognitive impairment increases early in the course of kidney disease, and several studies suggest CKD increases the risk for cognitive decline independent of age, diabetes and hypertension [3, 4]. However, the mechanisms leading to increased risk for cognitive decline among patients with CKD remain poorly understood.

In the general population, anemia is associated with increased risk for cognitive decline and dementia. In prospective studies, anemia has been associated with a 41–61 % increased risk for dementia [57]. Anemia is common among patients with CKD [8, 9], due in part to reduced erythropoietin production as kidney function declines. Some, but not all studies have identified a cross-sectional association between anemia and cognitive function in patients with ESRD. [2, 10] Previously, we found that lower hemoglobin (Hb) concentration was cross-sectionally associated with a higher prevalence of cognitive impairment using a single measure of cognitive function in the Chronic Renal Insufficiency Cohort (CRIC), a large, racially diverse cohort of adults with CKD [11]. Prospective studies of the association between anemia and change in cognitive function in patients with CKD are lacking.

We examined whether anemia was independently correlated with cross-sectional and longitudinal measures of cognitive function in adults with CKD. To address this question, we used data collected in the CRIC study, which included annual comprehensive cognitive assessments in a subset of older adults.

Methods

Study design and recruitment

The CRIC Study is a prospective observational cohort study designed to evaluate risk factors for progression of CKD among adults with moderate to advanced CKD. The study design, methods and baseline characteristics of study participants have been previously described [12, 13]. From June 2003 through May 2008, we recruited 3939 persons aged 21–74 years from seven clinical centers across the United States. Participants met age-based estimated glomerular filtration rate (eGFR) criteria: 20–70 ml/min/1.73 m2 for ages 21–44 years, 20–60 ml/min/1.73 m2 for ages 45–64 years, and 20–50 ml/min/1.73 m2 for ages 65–74 years. Exclusion criteria included diagnosis of polycystic kidney disease, pregnancy, recent immunosuppression for kidney disease, coexisting disease likely to affect survival, prior receipt of dialysis or organ transplant, or institutionalization (including residence in nursing homes).

Ethics, consent and permissions

Institutional Review Boards at all clinical sites approved the study protocol and all participants provided written informed consent (Additional file 1).

Assessment of cognitive function

Beginning in 2006, 825 participants aged 55 or older from four of the seven CRIC clinical centers were enrolled in an ancillary study (the CRIC COG Study). They underwent an annual battery of the six following cognitive function tests [14]. The Modified Mini-Mental State Exam (3MS) is a test of global cognitive function with components for concentration, orientation, language, praxis, and memory [15]. The Trailmaking Test (Trails) A measures attention, visuospatial scanning, and motor speed. The Trailmaking Test (Trails) B primarily assesses executive function [16]. Category Fluency evaluates verbal production, semantic memory, and language [17]. The Buschke Selective Reminding Test measures verbal memory with delayed components [18]. The Boston Naming assesses language function by asking participants to name objects presented in pictures [17].

Assessment of anemia and other covariates

At the baseline study visit and each annual visit thereafter, a complete blood count was performed. For these analyses, we used the Hb measurement corresponding to the first cognitive battery assessment. Anemia was defined according to the World Health Organization criteria of Hb <13 g/dL in men or Hb <12 g/dL in women [19]. Red cell mean corpuscular volume was categorized as <80, 80–99, and ≥100 fL for descriptive purposes.

Socio-demographic and clinical characteristics were assessed at the baseline visit and updated annually. We defined diabetes by participant self-report, use of medications for diabetes, or fasting blood glucose of ≥126 mg/dL. We defined hypertension by participant self-report, use of medications for high blood pressure, or a seated blood pressure of ≥140/80 mm Hg. We defined coronary heart disease as participant self-report of a myocardial infarction, angina or coronary revascularization procedure. We defined cerebrovascular disease as participant self-report of a stroke. We defined peripheral vascular disease as participant self-report of claudication, amputation, or revascularization procedure of the extremities. We defined alcohol use as participant self-report of current or former use of beer, wine or liquor. Erythropoietin use in the previous 30 days was ascertained by medication review conducted by CRIC Study personnel. Serum creatinine concentration and cystatin C concentration were measured annually at the central study laboratory. We calculated the estimated glomerular filtration rate (eGFR) with an equation derived from the CRIC Study, using the annual serum creatinine and cystatin C measurements corresponding to the first cognitive function assessment [20].

Statistical analyses

Continuous variables were expressed as means ± standard deviations (SDs) and compared using t-tests. Categorical variables were expressed as proportions and compared using the chi-squared test. We defined clinically significant cognitive impairment for each test separately, as a score one standard deviation (SD) or worse than the mean [21]. We estimated the association between anemia and baseline cognitive impairment using logistic models. The first model adjusted for demographic characteristics - age, sex, race, education and CRIC clinical center. The second model adjusted for demographic characteristics in addition to chronic health conditions -diabetes, hypertension, coronary artery disease, peripheral vascular disease, stroke, and alcohol use. The third model additionally adjusted for eGFR.

Next, we determined the association between anemia at baseline and annualized visit-to-visit changes in cognitive scores on each test, using mixed effects models with unstructured residual correlation matrix to account for within-subject correlation of the repeated changes. Cognitive assessments obtained after the development of ESRD were not included in longitudinal models. We constructed three models, as we did for the cross-sectional analysis, sequentially adjusting for baseline cognitive function and demographic characteristics, chronic health conditions, and eGFR. In sensitivity analyses, we excluded individuals receiving erythropoietin stimulating agents for treatment of anemia. We also conducted sensitivity analyses evaluating Hb as a continuous measure. All analyses were conducted using SAS 9.4 (Cary, NC).

Results

Of the 825 participants enrolled in the CRIC COG study, 26 were missing baseline Hb values and 37 were missing key covariates, resulting in an analytic cohort of 762 participants. The sample had a mean age of 64.4 ± 5.6 years and mean eGFR of 42.7 ± 16.4 ml/min/1.73 m2. Anemia was present in 349 (46 %) of participants. Compared to participants without anemia, participants with anemia were more likely to be black and have lower educational attainment (Table 1). In addition, participants with anemia were more likely to have diabetes, hypertension, coronary artery disease, peripheral vascular disease, and stroke, and less likely to currently use alcohol. Participants with anemia had lower eGFR and higher levels of albuminuria. The majority of participants with anemia had a mean corpuscular volume in the normocytic range (80–99 fL). There were 50 participants (6.6 %) receiving erythropoietin stimulating agents, including 38 (10.9 %) participants with anemia. The distribution of Hb values stratified by sex is shown in Fig. 1.

Table 1 Baseline characteristics of CRIC participants stratified by anemia status
Fig. 1
figure 1

Distribution of hemoglobin concentration among 762 participants age ≥55 in the Chronic Renal Insufficiency Cohort

In analyses adjusted for demographic characteristics, anemia was associated with higher odds of cognitive impairment on the Trails A at baseline, but not significantly associated with impairment on other cognitive tests (Table 2). After adjustment for age, sex, race, education, CRIC clinical center, diabetes, hypertension, coronary artery disease, peripheral vascular disease, stroke, alcohol use, and eGFR, there was no significant association between anemia and cognitive impairment on any of the six cognitive measures.

Table 2 Association of anemia with baseline cognitive impairment among participants age ≥55 in the Chronic Renal Insufficiency Cohort

There were 70 participants who did not complete any follow-up cognitive assessments. The remaining 692 participants were followed for a median of 2.9 years (IQR 2.6–3.0). There was no significant association between anemia and change in cognitive function on the 3MS, Buschke Delayed Recall, Boston Naming and Trails A tests in the partially adjusted or fully adjusted models (Table 3). After adjustment for baseline cognitive function, age, sex, race, education and CRIC clinical center, there was a significant association between anemia and more pronounced decline on the Trails B test, and between anemia and less pronounced decline on the Category fluency test (Table 3). After additional adjustment for chronic health conditions the associations between anemia and change in the Trails B and Category Fluency tests were attenuated and no longer significant. Further adjustment for eGFR did not change these findings.

Table 3 Adjusted mean annual change in cognitive scores between anemic vs non-anemic participants. A positive parameter estimate denotes a larger decline in cognitive function for the Modified Mini-Mental State Exam, Category Fluency, Buschke Delayed Recall and Boston Naming tests. For the Trailmaking Test A and B, a negative parameter estimate denotes a larger decline in cognitive function

In sensitivity analyses excluding individuals receiving erythropoietin stimulating agents, the results were similar. For example, the fully adjusted association between anemia and change in the Category Fluency test was −0.10 ± 0.27 (p-value = 0.22), and between anemia and change in the Trails B test was −0.71 ± 3.98 (p-value 0.27). Similarly, we found no significant association between Hb, evaluated as a continuous measure, with changes in cognitive function in fully adjusted models.

Discussion

In a well-characterized cohort of older adults with CKD studied prospectively, we did not find an independent association between anemia and baseline cognitive function or the rate of cognitive decline over nearly 3 years. Our findings were consistent across multiple domains of cognitive function including global cognition, executive function, language and memory. These results do not support the hypothesis that anemia is an independent risk factor for cognitive decline in older persons with CKD.

There is limited information regarding the relationship between anemia and cognitive function in patients with CKD. In the full CRIC cohort of more than 3500 participants, we previously reported a cross-sectional association between Hb, evaluated as a continuous measure, and impairment on the 3MS, a measure of global cognition. Most previous studies of CKD and anemia were conducted in patients with ESRD receiving maintenance dialysis, and many of these studies were conducted shortly after the introduction of erythropoietin for treatment of anemia. Early studies posited that treatment of anemia with erythropoietin might have salutary effects on brain function among patients with ESRD receiving dialysis. Most of these studies evaluated brain evoked potentials as an end-point; however a few also assessed cognitive function. For example, in an uncontrolled study of 24 patients, treatment with erythropoietin over 12 months increased hematocrit from 23.7 to 36.0 %, and was associated with an improvement in attention and executive function [22]. The absence of a control group in these studies limits the ability to distinguish learning effects from a causal effect of anemia treatment. Contemporary studies conducted in hemodialysis patients with less severe anemia, most of whom were receiving treatment with erythropoietin, have not found a strong relation between anemia and cognitive function. In one study of 338 patients with ESRD assessed with a cognitive battery, Hb <11 g/dl was associated with cognitive impairment in unadjusted analyses, but not in adjusted analyses [2]. In a cross-sectional study of 109 patients with ESRD, unadjusted Hb concentration was correlated with global cognition, but not with executive function [10]. In another cross-sectional study of 383 patients with ESRD, there was no association between Hb concentration and global cognition or executive function [23].

Compared to prior studies, the current study utilized a longitudinal study design. Although anemia was common in this cohort, treatment with erythropoietin was infrequent, reflective of current practice guidelines [24]. There are several possible explanations why anemia might be associated with dementia and cognitive decline in the general population [57], but not among patients with CKD. Compared to studies in the general population, the prevalence of anemia in the CRIC Study cohort was higher, but the follow-up period was shorter, and this may have been insufficient to identify clinically significant changes in cognitive function. We censored patients at ESRD to eliminate the potential confounding effects introduced by the changes in health status and treatment of anemia surrounding this critical transition period. It is possible that the risk for cognitive decline is increased only for patients with severe anemia, that is, a Hb concentration well below the WHO definition. This seems unlikely, however, since prior studies in the general population also utilized the WHO definition of anemia.

Another possible explanation of our findings is that the association between anemia and cognitive impairment is confounded by nutritional deficiencies, such as B12 deficiency, and that these nutritional deficiencies are a more common cause of anemia in the general population. We lacked measures of B12, folate, and iron stores in the current study. Nevertheless, the majority of participants in the CRIC COG Study had a normocytic anemia, which would not be typical for isolated B12 or iron deficiency anemia. Erythropoietin deficiency has also been suggested as a mechanistic link between anemia and cognitive decline, based on animal models demonstrating erythropoietin receptors in the brain and uncontrolled clinical studies of patients treated with erythropoietin stimulating agents. Given that erythropoietin deficiency plays a major role in the anemia of CKD, this pathway seems unlikely to be involved based on the results of the current study. Finally, it may be that anemia is not causally related to cognitive decline in patients with CKD, but that it is a marker for other conditions which affect cognition. This conclusion is suggested by the observation that demographic and clinical characteristics largely attenuated the association between anemia and baseline cognitive function and cognitive decline.

Our study has several strengths, including the multi-center, prospective design, diverse study population, use of a cognitive battery to assess cognitive function, and ability to account for a number of important confounding factors. We acknowledge several limitations. First, the follow-up period was short. Second, our participants (volunteers for a long-term research study) may be at lower risk for cognitive decline than the broader population with CKD because they are selected for the ability to comply with study visits and they may be more motivated to pursue healthy behaviors. Both of these factors may have limited the power to detect changes in cognitive function and biased our study towards the null. Thus, a more subtle relationship between anemia and cognitive function may have gone undetected because of a type II error. We also lacked information about nutritional factors associated with anemia such as B12, folate, and iron.

Conclusions

Although anemia was common among older adults with CKD, it was not independently associated with baseline cognitive function or the rate of cognitive decline. For now, the mechanisms contributing to cognitive decline in patients with CKD remain elusive.

Abbreviations

CKD:

chronic kidney disease

CRIC:

chronic renal insufficiency cohort

eGFR:

estimated glomerular filtration rate

ESRD:

end stage renal disease

Hb:

hemoglobin

References

  1. Kurella Tamura M, Wadley V, Yaffe K, McClure LA, Howard G, Go R, et al. Kidney function and cognitive impairment in US adults: the Reasons for Geographic and Racial Differences in Stroke (REGARDS) Study. Am J Kidney Dis. 2008;52(2):227–34.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Murray AM, Tupper DE, Knopman DS, Gilbertson DT, Pederson SL, Li S, et al. Cognitive impairment in hemodialysis patients is common. Neurology. 2006;67(2):216–23.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Seliger SL, Siscovick DS, Stehman-Breen CO, Gillen DL, Fitzpatrick A, Bleyer A, et al. Moderate renal impairment and risk of dementia among older adults: the Cardiovascular Health Cognition Study. J Am Soc Nephrol. 2004;15(7):1904–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Kurella M, Chertow GM, Fried LF, Cummings SR, Harris T, Simonsick E, et al. Chronic kidney disease and cognitive impairment in the elderly: the health, aging, and body composition study. J Am Soc Nephrol. 2005;16(7):2127–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Atti AR, Palmer K, Volpato S, Zuliani G, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. Anaemia increases the risk of dementia in cognitively intact elderly. Neurobiol Aging. 2006;27(2):278–84.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Shah RC, Buchman AS, Wilson RS, Leurgans SE, Bennett DA. Hemoglobin level in older persons and incident Alzheimer disease: prospective cohort analysis. Neurology. 2011;77(3):219–26.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Hong CH, Falvey C, Harris TB, Simonsick EM, Satterfield S, Ferrucci L, et al. Anemia and risk of dementia in older adults: findings from the Health ABC study. Neurology. 2013;81(6):528–33.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Astor BC, Muntner P, Levin A, Eustace JA, Coresh J. Association of kidney function with anemia: the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (1988–1994). Arch Intern Med. 2002;162(12):1401–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Hsu CY, McCulloch CE, Curhan GC. Epidemiology of anemia associated with chronic renal insufficiency among adults in the United States: results from the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. J Am Soc Nephrol. 2002;13(2):504–10.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Leinau L, Murphy TE, Bradley E, Fried T. Relationship between conditions addressed by hemodialysis guidelines and non-ESRD-specific conditions affecting quality of life. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. 2009;4(3):572–8.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Kurella Tamura M, Xie D, Yaffe K, Cohen DL, Teal V, Kasner SE, et al. Vascular risk factors and cognitive impairment in chronic kidney disease: the Chronic Renal Insufficiency Cohort (CRIC) study. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. 2011;6(2):248–56.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Feldman HI, Appel LJ, Chertow GM, Cifelli D, Cizman B, Daugirdas J, et al. The Chronic Renal Insufficiency Cohort (CRIC) study: design and methods. J Am Soc Nephrol. 2003;14(7 Suppl 2):S148–153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Lash JP, Go AS, Appel LJ, He J, Ojo A, Rahman M, et al. Chronic Renal Insufficiency Cohort (CRIC) study: baseline characteristics and associations with kidney function. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. 2009;4(8):1302–11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Yaffe K, Ackerson L, Kurella Tamura M, Le Blanc P, Kusek JW, Sehgal AR, et al. Chronic Renal Insufficiency Cohort I. Chronic kidney disease and cognitive function in older adults: findings from the chronic renal insufficiency cohort cognitive study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2010;58(2):338–45.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Teng EL, Chui HC. The Modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) examination. J Clin Psychiatry. 1987;48(8):314–8.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Reitan RM. Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept Mot Skills. 1958;8:271–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Welsh KA, Butters N, Mohs RC, Beekly D, Edland S, Fillenbaum G, et al. The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). Part V. A normative study of the neuropsychological battery. Neurology. 1994;44(4):609–14.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Buschke H, Fuld PA. Evaluating storage, retention, and retrieval in disordered memory and learning. Neurology. 1974;24(11):1019–25.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. DeMaeyer E, Adiels-Tegman M. The prevalence of anaemia in the world. World Health Stat Q. 1985;38(3):302–16.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Anderson AH, Yang W, Hsu CY, Joffe MM, Leonard MB, Xie D, et al. Estimating GFR among participants in the Chronic Renal Insufficiency Cohort (CRIC) Study. Am J Kidney Dis. 2012;60(2):250–61.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Hayden KM, Khachaturian AS, Tschanz JT, Corcoran C, Nortond M, Breitner JC. Characteristics of a two-stage screen for incident dementia. J Clin Epidemiol. 2003;56(11):1038–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Marsh JT, Brown WS, Wolcott D, Carr CR, Harper R, Schweitzer SV, et al. rHuEPO treatment improves brain and cognitive function of anemic dialysis patients. Kidney Int. 1991;39(1):155–63.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Kurella Tamura M, Larive B, Unruh ML, Stokes JB, Nissenson A, Mehta RL, et al. Prevalence and correlates of cognitive impairment in hemodialysis patients: the Frequent Hemodialysis Network trials. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. 2010;5(8):1429–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Kliger AS, Foley RN, Goldfarb DS, Goldstein SL, Johansen K, Singh A, et al. KDOQI US commentary on the 2012 KDIGO Clinical Practice Guideline for Anemia in CKD. Am J Kidney Dis. 2013;62(5):849–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Funding for the CRIC Study was obtained under a cooperative agreement from National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases (U01DK060990, U01DK060984, U01DK061022, U01DK061021, U01DK061028, U01DK060980, U01DK060963, and U01DK060902). In addition, this work was supported in part by: the University of Pennsylvania CTRC CTSA UL1 RR-024134, Johns Hopkins University UL1 RR-025005, University of Maryland GCRC M01 RR-16500, Clinical and Translational Science Collaborative of Cleveland, UL1TR000439 from the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (NCATS) component of the National Institutes of Health and NIH roadmap for Medical Research, Michigan Institute for Clinical and Health Research (MICHR) UL1RR024986, University of Illinois at Chicago CTSA UL1RR029879, Tulane University Translational Research in Hypertension and Renal Biology P30GM103337, Kaiser NIH/NCRR UCSF-CTSI UL1 RR-024131.

The CRIC COG study is supported by R01DK069406 from the NIDDK.

*CRIC Study Investigators:

Lawrence J. Appel, MD, MPH

Harold I. Feldman, MD, MSCE

Alan S. Go, MD

Jiang He, MD, PhD

John W. Kusek, PhD

James P. Lash, MD

Akinlolu Ojo, MD, PhD

Mahboob Rahman, MD

Raymond R. Townsend, MD

Views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the Department of Veterans Affairs

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Manjula Kurella Tamura.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests

Authors’ contributions

MKT contributed to the conception, design, acquisition and interpretation of the data, and drafting of the manuscript. EV supervised the statistical analysis and contributed to interpretation of the data. JY conducted the statistical analysis. AG contributed to the acquisition and interpretation of data and revision of the manuscript for intellectual content. SS contributed to the acquisition and interpretation of data and revision of the manuscript for intellectual content. JK contributed to interpretation of data and revision of the manuscript for intellectual content. JL contributed to the acquisition and interpretation of data and revision of the manuscript for intellectual content. DC contributed to the acquisition and interpretation of data and revision of the manuscript for intellectual content. JS contributed to the interpretation of data and revision of the manuscript for intellectual content. VB contributed to the interpretation of data and revision of the manuscript for intellectual content. JO contributed to the interpretation of data and revision of the manuscript for intellectual content. GM contributed to the interpretation of data and revision of the manuscript for intellectual content. KY contributed to the conception, design, acquisition and interpretation of the data, and revision of the manuscript for intellectual content. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Additional file

Additional file 1:

Supplemental File. (DOCX 12 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kurella Tamura, M., Vittinghoff, E., Yang, J. et al. Anemia and risk for cognitive decline in chronic kidney disease. BMC Nephrol 17, 13 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12882-016-0226-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12882-016-0226-6

Keywords