Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Patterns and characteristics of cognitive functioning in older patients approaching end stage kidney disease, the COPE-study

Abstract

Background

The prevalence of impaired cognitive functioning in older patients with end stage kidney disease (ESKD) is high. We aim to describe patterns of memory, executive function or psychomotor speed and to identify nephrologic, geriatric and neuroradiologic characteristics associated with cognitive impairment in older patients approaching ESKD who have not yet started with renal replacement therapy (RRT).

Methods

The COPE-study (Cognitive Decline in Older Patients with ESRD) is a prospective cohort study including 157 participants aged 65 years and older approaching ESKD (eGFR ≤20 ml/min/1.73 m2) prior to starting with RRT. In addition to routinely collected clinical parameters related to ESKD, such as vascular disease burden and parameters of metabolic disturbance, patients received a full geriatric assessment, including extensive neuropsychological testing. In a subgroup of patients (n = 93) a brain MRI was performed.

Results

The median age was 75.3 years. Compared to the normative data of neuropsychological testing participants memory performance was in the 24th percentile, executive function in the 18th percentile and psychomotor speed in the 20th percentile. Independent associated characteristics of impairment in memory, executive and psychomotor speed were high age, low educational level and low functional status (all p-values < 0.003). A history of vascular disease (p = 0.007) and more white matter hyperintensities on brain MRI (p = 0.013) were associated with a lower psychomotor speed.

Conclusion

Older patients approaching ESKD have a high prevalence of impaired memory, executive function and psychomotor speed. The patterns of cognitive impairment and brain changes on MRI are suggestive of vascular cognitive impairment. These findings could be of potentially added value in the decision-making process concerning patients with ESKD.

Peer Review reports

Background

Older patients approaching end stage kidney disease (ESKD) have, compared to younger patients, an increased risk for adverse health outcomes in general [1] and for impaired cognitive functioning [2], with a high prevalence ranging from 30 to 87% in dialysis patients [3, 4]. Cognitive impairment has a major impact on outcomes in (older) patients receiving renal replacement therapy (RRT) [5]. Understanding patterns and associated characteristics of cognitive functioning in pre-dialysis and prior to initiating RRT may guide informed treatment decisions and ultimately minimize the risk for further cognitive decline.

Several pathophysiological mechanisms have been suggested as factor contributing to the high prevalence of impaired cognitive function in patients approaching ESKD such as vascular, neurodegenerative and metabolic processes [6, 7], [8]. Both the brain and kidney are low resistance end organs, exposed to high blood flow and vulnerable to vascular damage [9]. If vascular damage plays a role in developing kidney disease, this may also affect the cerebral vasculature, leading to structural brain abnormalities and cognitive impairment, predominantly in the executive domains and psychomotor speed [10]. Accumulation of uremic toxins may cause cerebral endothelial dysfunction, and lead to neurodegenerative damage in brain regions that play a dominant role in cognitive domains of attention and speed [11]. Only a few studies report on the systematic assessment of patterns of cognitive functioning and their determinants in older patients approaching ESKD prior to treatment initiation. Importantly information on the actual brain damage observed on brain MRI is scarce [12]. This timepoint is of particular importance, as these cognitive impairments may influence treatment decisions and treatment outcome.

In the COPE study [13] we aimed to describe patterns of memory, executive function or psychomotor speed and to identify nephrologic, geriatric and neuroradiologic characteristics associated with cognitive impairment in older patients approaching ESKD who have not yet started with renal replacement therapy RRT.

Methods

Study design

The full design of the COPE study, methods and rationale have been published previously [13]. In brief, the COPE study is a prospective, multicentre cohort study in five hospitals in the Netherlands in patients aged 65 years and older reaching ESRD (estimated glomerular filtration rate (eGFR) ≤ 20 ml/min/1.73 m2), and attending the pre-dialysis outpatient between April 2014 and December 2017. As part of routine pre-dialysis nephro-geriatric work-up, a comprehensive geriatric assessment (CGA), physical examination, laboratory investigation, neuropsychological testing and a brain MRI scan (in case there was no contra-indication) were performed. The study protocol was approved by the medical ethics committee (METC) of all participating centres.

Routine renal care

As part of routine pre-dialysis nephrogeriatric work-up, the following clinical parameters were measured: kidney function, metabolic state (urea, phosphate, calcium) and parameters on vascular status (blood pressure, ankle/arm index). eGFR was estimated glomerular filtration rate using the Modified of Diet in Renal Disease (MDRD) [14] or Chronic Kidney Disease Epidemiology Collaboration (CKD-epi) [15] depending on the method used in the different hospitals. Patients were allocated into two groups, vascular and non-vascular cause of kidney disease according to the ERA-EDTA primary renal diagnosis code as assessed by the treating nephrologist. Vascular disease burden was determined as the cause of kidney disease (vascular versus non-vascular), ankle-brachial index, the presence of diabetes and a history of vascular disease (myocardial infarction and/or cerebral vascular incident (CVA) and/or peripheral vascular disease). We considered serum urea, phosphate and calcium as parameters of metabolic disturbance.

Geriatric work-up

As part of the nephro-geriatric work-up, all patients underwent a comprehensive geriatric assessment (CGA). For a more detailed description of the tests used in the COPE study, see the previously published study protocol [13]. Briefly, the CGA work-up included the following tests: nutrition was assessed using the Normal Subjective Global Assessment score (SGA) [16] and the Short Nutritional Assessment Questionnaire score (SNAQ) [17]. Frailty was assessed using the Fried Frailty Index (FFI) and a score of ≥3 was considered as frail [18]. Functional dependence was assessed by the Groningen Activity Restriction Scale (GARS), where higher scores are indicative of increased dependence (range 18–72) [19], and The Lawton Instrumental Activity of Daily Living score (IADL), with a score ≥ 11 being described as functional dependency [20]. Furthermore, to assess physical capacity the handgrip strength and 6-m gait speed were measured.

Neuropsychological testing

Trained geriatric or dialysis nurses administered a standardized neuropsychological test battery. It was designed to assess different domains of cognitive functioning such as global cognition, visuoconstruction, memory, executive function and psychomotor speed. The test battery has been successfully used in several study cohorts over the past 20 years [21,22,23] and is based on clinical experience, scientific literature and relevance for clinical interference [21]. To test global cognition the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) was used, with scores ranging from 0 to 30 points where higher scores are indicative of better cognitive performance [24]. Clock drawing was used to assess visuoconstructive abilities and executive function, with scores ranging from 0 to 14 points and higher scores are indicative of better performance [25, 26]. Memory, was assessed using the 15-Word Verbal Learning Test (WVLT) both immediately (total score after five trials) and delayed recall (total score after 20 min), where higher scores indicatedbetter function [27]. To assess memory reproduction the Visual Attention Test (VAT) was used, where higher scores indicated better function [28]. Executive function was assessed with visual attention and task switching was tested with the Trail Making Test A and B (TMT-A and TMT-B), with lower scores indicating better function [29]. To distinguish between processing speed or cognitive (in) flexibility as an explanation of the test result the score on the TMT-B was corrected for the score on the TMT-A. In addition the Stroop Colour Word Test (SCWT) was used, where lower scores indicated better function [30], [31]. To distinguish between processing speed and cognitive inhibition as an explanation of the test result the score on the Stroop III (interference card) was corrected for the score on the Stroop II (colour naming card). To assess psychomotor speed the Letter Digit Substitution Test (LDST), the Stroop II and the TMT-A was used. For the LDST the number of correct substitutions made in 60 s was used, with higher scores indicating better function [32].

Normative data of neuropsychological testing

To compare the cognitive test results of the current study with a general population, Dutch normative data for neuropsychological tests corrected for age, gender and educational level were used this to interpret the cognitive status independent of age, gender and level of education [33]. These normative data are frequently used for clinical ratings in daily practice and were available for the 15-WVLT, TMT-A, TMT-B and the SCWT. The norms were based on between 300 and 1000 healthy participants aged 14–90 years.

MRI of the brain

As part of routine nephro-geriatric work-up a brain MRI was performed in all patients without a contra-indication for MRI. Contraindications for not performing a MRI were: having a pacemaker, having a metallic foreign body (metal sliver) in their eye, being claustrophobic, or not being able to have access to the MRI table due to backpain or impaired mobility. Brain MRI scans were acquired on a Philips Ingenia 3 T scanners at the LUMC (Philips Medical Systems, Best, The Netherlands) according to a standardized scanning protocol. The scanning protocol included T1-weighted images (repetition time (TR) = 8.2 ms; echo time (TE) = 4.5 ms; flip angle 8°, voxel size 1x1x1mm3), fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) images (TR = 4800 ms; TE = 313 ms; inversion time (TI) = 1650 ms; voxel size 1.11 × 1.11 × 1.11mm3) and susceptibility-weighted imaging (TR = 45 ms; TE 31 ms; flip angle 13°; voxel size 0.8 × 0.8 × 1.6mm3). The brain MRI scans were scored for markers of small vessel disease (white matter hyperintensities) and lacunes of presumed vascular origin and microbleeds) according to the STRIVE criteria [34]. White matter hyperintensities were assessed by the Scheltens scale [35].

Statistical methods

Baseline characteristics are presented as mean with standard deviation (SD) in case of normal distribution, median with interquartile range (IQR) in case of skewed distribution or as number (n) with percentages (%). Mean functioning on the different cognitive domains (memory, executive function and psychomotor speed) are presented as percentiles (mean with IQR), according to the normative data neuropsychological testing (see above). To assess associated characteristics of cognitive functioning in different domains, different cognitive tests are stratified in tertiles and mean scores of the different associated characteristics are calculated over the tertiles of cognitive functioning, presented as mean (standard error (SE)). Crude and adjusted p-values were calculated with univariable and multivariable linear regression models, respectively, with the continuous score of cognitive performance as dependent variable. In multivariable models we adjusted for age, gender and educational level, to ensure a balanced comparison between the tertiles. The MRI abnormalities were also assessed as determinant of cognitive function. The p-values are presented crude and adjusted (again for age, gender and educational level). All analyses were carried out using SPSS (IBM version 23; IBM Corp., Armonk, New York, USA).

Results

Table 1 shows the baseline characteristics of the study population. The study population consisted of 157 participants with a median age of 75 years and 103 (66%) participants were male. At study enrolment, the mean eGFR was 16.2 ml/min/1.73m2 (standard deviation (SD) 4.4) and over the past 3 years the mean decline in eGFR was 9.1 ml/min/1.73m2 (SD 8.0). In 99 (63%) patients a vascular cause, mainly hypertension or diabetes mellitus, was the cause of their primary kidney disease. Almost half of the participants (n = 74; 47%) had a history of vascular disease. According to the Fried Frailty Index (FFI) 37 (25%) patients were frail. Functional dependence, as measured by the Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (IADL) with a score of ≥11, was found in 8 (5%) of the patients.

Table 1 Baseline characteristics of the included study population

Performance on the global cognitive function and different cognitive domains are reported in the Supplemental Table 1. The population had a median Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) of 28 out of 30 points (IQR 27–29). Mean functioning on the memory test (15-Word Verbal Learning Test (15-WVLT)) was in the 24th percentile (IQR 10–54) with a mean score of 31.2 words remembered (SD 9.9). The mean functioning on the executive function (Trail Making Test B (TMT-B)) was in the 18th percentile (IQR 3–54) with a mean score 177.4 s (SD 79.5). The mean functioning on psychomotor speed (Letter Digit Substitution Test (LDST) was in the 20th percentile (IQR 10–50) with a mean score of 21.7 correct substitutions (SD 6.9).

In Tables 2 and 3 and in Supplemental Table 2 we report the associated characteristics of three different cognitive domains, namely memory, executive function and psychomotor speed, respectively. In all three cognitive domains, as expected, older age and lower level of education were significantly associated with cognitive impairment (all p-values ≤0.007). For example, the patients who performed in the worst tertile in memory function, compared to the best tertile, were on average 5 years older (p < 0.001) and more often received a lower educational level (for memory function: 20% versus 33%, p = 0.001).

Table 2 Associated characteristics of memory function
Table 3 Associated characteristics of executive function

Table 2 shows the associated characteristics of the memory domain. After adjusting for age, gender and educational level a higher level of functional dependence (IADL-score) was significantly associated with a more impaired memory function (p = 0.003). Patients who performed in the worst tertile of memory function were more functionally dependent compared to the patients who performed in the best tertile (mean IADL-score of 4.6 (SE 0.6) versus a mean IADL-score 2.0 (SE 0.4); p < 0.003). Having a history of vascular disease was associated with a more impaired memory function, although the association lost statistical significance after adjustment for age, gender and educational level. Parameters of metabolic disturbance were not associated with an impaired memory function.

Table 3 presents the associated characteristics of the cognitive domain of executive function. After adjusting for age, gender and educational level, a higher level of functional dependence (p < 0.001), the presence of frailty (p = 0.001) and a lower handgrip strength (p = 0.020) were significantly associated with a more impaired executive functioning. For example, in the tertile with the worst executive function, the presence of frailty was higher compared to the best tertile (mean Fried Frailty Index of 2.1 (SE 0.2) versus a mean Fried Frailty Index 1.0 (SE 0.2); p = 0.001). Having a history of vascular disease associated with an impaired executive function, although the association lost statistical significance after adjustment for age, gender and educational level. Parameters of metabolic disturbance were not associated with an impaired executive function.

Supplemental Table 2 presents the associated characteristics on the cognitive domain of psychomotor speed. After adjusting for age, gender and educational level, a higher presence of frailty (p = 0.001), a higher level of functional dependence (p < 0.001) and a lower handgrip strength (p = 0.026) were significantly associated with impaired performance on psychomotor speed. For example, the patients who performed in the worst tertile of psychomotor speed had a lower handgrip strength compared to the patients who performed in the best tertile (mean handgrip strength of 24.9 (SE 1.3) versus a mean handgrip strength 26.8 (SE 1.4); p = 0.026). After adjusting for age, gender and educational level, having a history of vascular disease was associated with an impaired performance on psychomotor speed (p = 0.007). Again, parameters of metabolic disturbance were not associated with an impaired performance psychomotor speed.

The cerebrovascular MRI features in a subpopulation (n = 93) are presented in Supplemental Table 3. When comparing the patients with and without a MRI performed, we see that the patients without a MRI are older, more frail, more functional dependent and a higher history of vascular disease, although not statistically significant possibly due to small sample size. The mean Scheltens score of the white matter hyperintensities was 15.8 (SD 7.6). Lobar microbleeds were present in 37 (40%) of the included participants and 19 (20%) participants had non-lobar microbleeds. Lacunes of presumed vascular origin were present in 44 (48%) participants. Table 4 shows which brain MRI abnormalities are associated characteristics of the different neuropsychological domains memory, executive function and psychomotor speed. When adjusting for age, gender and educational level, only a higher burden of white matter hyperintensities was significantly associated with worse psychomotor speed. Patients who performed in the worst tertile of psychomotor speed on average had more white matter hyperintensities compared to patients who performed in the best tertile (mean white matter hyperintensities of 18.6 (SE 1.6) versus a mean white matter hyperintensities 14.6 (SE 1.2); p = 0.013). A trend was observed for the association between a higher burden of white matter hyperintensities and lower executive function scores (p = 0.054).

Table 4 Association between brain MRI features with domains of cognitive function

Discussion

The main findings of the present study were twofold. Firstly, impaired cognitive function, in the domains of memory, executive function and psychomotor speed was highly prevalent in patients approaching ESKD not yet started with RRT. Secondly, associated characteristics of a worse cognitive function in the domains memory, executive and psychomotor speed were older age, lower level education, lower functional status, frailty, higher burden of white matter hyperintensities on MRI and a history of vascular disease. Surprisingly, cognitive function was not influenced by parameters of metabolic disturbance.

In the present study, older patients approaching ESKD performed worse on all cognitive domains tested in comparison to the general population. This is consistent with findings in a study of younger patients attending a pre-dialysis clinic in where impairments in psychomotor efficiency and processing speed were more evident than impairments in the domains of learning efficiency or attention and working memory [36]. Similar to our findings one other study [37] of older patients with chronic kidney disease (N = 385 median creatinine clearance of 19 ml/min) reported deficits in all cognitive domains, with the largest deficiencies found in recall, attention and executive function. We observed that associated characteristics of a worse cognitive function in the domains memory, executive and psychomotor speed were older age, lower education, lower functional status, frailty, higher burden of white matter hyperintensities on MRI and a history of vascular disease. In other populations with CKD, age, history of falls, functional status and a history of vascular disease were previously described as determinants associated with impaired cognition [6, 37]. A number of studies have reported the prevalence of geriatric impairments, such as dependency in activities of daily living (ADLs) and cognitive impairment, in younger patients with ESKD [38, 39]. The association between white matter hyperintensities and an impaired cognitive function, particularly impairment in attention, executive function and information processing speed, has also been described in older community dwelling and hospitalised patients [40,41,42]. In our study, parameters of metabolic disturbance (urea, phosphate, calcium) were not associated with a worse cognitive function. There were conflicting results reported on the association of metabolic determinants and the association with a worse cognitive function [11, 43]. In summary, the patterns and associated characteristics of cognitive impairment and the neuroradiological findings in our study population are in line with the previous limited literature.

There are several possible pathophysiological mechanisms that could explain the patterns and associated characteristics of cognitive impairment and the neuroradiological findings in older patients with ESKD described in our study. Firstly, it is possible that ESKD and cerebral vascular damage, are endpoints of the same pathophysiological pathway. Both the brain and kidney share similar vascular anatomy, as low resistance end organs exposed to high volume blood flow into their small vessels, and both have an auto-regulatory system. Because of this unique system, small vessels in kidney and brain, both afferent arterioles and deep perforating arterioles, are particularly prone to be injured by systemic hypertension and other vascular disease [44] as well as by damage due to endothelial dysfunction. Small vessel disease can affect both the kidney and the brain and white matter hyperintensities are considered as a neuroradiological marker for small vessel disease. This could explain the correlation between an impaired renal function and MRI markers of cerebral small vessel disease found in earlier studies [45]. However, extensive research on brain, perfusion and cardiac structure in older ESKD patients is scarce. Second, the high burden of vascular and metabolic morbidity in patients with ESKD lead to an increased biological age, resulting in different phenotypes such as premature vascular aging, muscle wasting, bone disease, cognitive dysfunction and frailty [39]. Taken together, the patterns of cognition and neuroradiological imaging are suggestive of vascular cognitive impairment in older patients with ESKD. Further research is needed to unravel the exact underlying pathophysiological mechanism.

Our results could have some clinical implications. When patients approach ESKD several treatment options, such as RRT including dialysis or transplantation or conservative treatment, are considered. When making treatment decisions, it is important to have insight into the cognitive function of the patient for several reasons. Firstly, cognitive impairment is independently associated with increased mortality, including in patients on RRT [4, 46]. Secondly, patients with cognitive impairment have in general a higher risk for adverse health outcomes such as delirium. Last but not least, it is known that an impaired cognitive functioning can affect decision-making capacity [47] and therefore awareness of cognitive, functional or psycho-social impairment, prior to making a decision on RRT, is of the utmost importance. Information from the geriatric assessment can be taken to the discussions on treatment choice with the patient and family enhancing tailored treatment options for each individual patient.

There were several limitations to the current study. The study is integrated into routine clinical care and possibly has some patient selection bias. For example, the MRI brain is only performed in 60% of the participants, possibly causing some bias. Since the patients not having a MRI where older, more frail, more functional dependent and had a higher history of vascular disease the results described in our manuscript are probably underestimated. It could also be that the patients in worse condition were less likely to participate, which could result in an underestimation of the observed prevalence of cognitive impairment. The study has a relatively small group, which could cause a lack of power. The present analysis reported the cross-sectional association between several associated characteristics and cognition as a consequence that a causal association could not be established. Because of the fact there was not one primary analyses, and this was a post-hoc analysis, we did not perform a formal power calculation. We were however confident that our study provides sensible power to detect clinically relevant findings, because we did find all the known associated characteristics of cognitive function to be statistically significantly associated with cognition in all domains: higher age, higher education level, and higher disabilities in instrumental activities of daily living. Despite these limitations to our knowledge this was the first study in which cognitive function is described so extensively in combination with brain MRI’s in an older population approaching ESKD. The patients included in this study all had an eGFR < 20 ml/min/1.73m2 and were not on RRT a study population that has previously only received limited scientific attention. Furthermore, this study focused exclusively on older patients (included median age of 75.3 (IQR 70.8–80.8)), a group whom very often do not participate in clinical trials due to exclusion criteria [48, 49]. With the limited exclusion criteria applied in the COPE-study, the included study population reflected the patients in daily clinical practice.

Conclusion

Older patients approaching ESKD have a high prevalence of impaired memory, executive function and psychomotor speed. The patterns of cognitive impairment and brain changes on MRI are suggestive of vascular cognitive impairment. These findings could be of potentially added value in the decision-making process concerning patients with ESKD.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are not publicly available, but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

CGA:

Comprehensive geriatric assessment

CKD-epi:

Chronic Kidney Disease Epidemiology Collaboration

COPE-study:

Cognitive functioning in Older Patients reaching End stage renal disease

CVA:

Cerebral vascular accident

eGFR:

Estimated glomerular filtration rate

ESKD:

End stage kidney disease

FFI:

Fried Frailty Index

FLAIR:

Fluid-attenuated inversion recovery

GARS:

Groningen Activity Restriction Scale

MDRD:

Modified of Diet in Renal Disease

IADL:

Lawton Instrumental Activity of Daily Living

IQR:

Interquartile range

LDST:

Letter Digit Substitution Test

METC:

Medical ethics committee

MMSE:

Mini Mental State Examination

RRT:

Renal replacement therapy

SCWT:

Stroop Colour Word Test

SD:

Standard deviation

SE:

Standard error

SGA:

Subjective Global Assessment

SNAQ:

Short Nutritional Assessment Questionnaire

TE:

Echo time

TMT:

Trail Making Test

TI:

Inversion time

TR:

Repetition time

WVLT:

15-Word Verbal Learning Test

VAT:

Visual Attention Test

References

  1. Grams ME, Yang W, Rebholz CM, Wang X, Porter AC, Inker LA, et al. Risks of adverse events in advanced CKD: the chronic renal insufficiency cohort (CRIC) study. Am J Kidney Dis. 2017;70(3):337–46.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Drew DA, Weiner DE, Tighiouart H, Duncan S, Gupta A, Scott T, et al. Cognitive decline and its risk factors in prevalent hemodialysis patients. Am J Kidney Dis. 2017;69(6):780–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Sarnak MJ, Tighiouart H, Scott TM, Lou KV, Sorensen EP, Giang LM, et al. Frequency of and risk factors for poor cognitive performance in hemodialysis patients. Neurology. 2013;80(5):471–80.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Griva K, Stygall J, Hankins M, Davenport A, Harrison M, Newman SP. Cognitive impairment and 7-year mortality in dialysis patients. Am J Kidney Dis. 2010;56(4):693–703.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Rakowski DA, Caillard S, Agodoa LY, Abbott KC. Dementia as a predictor of mortality in dialysis patients. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. 2006;1(5):1000–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Murray AM. Cognitive impairment in the aging dialysis and chronic kidney disease populations: an occult burden. Adv Chronic Kidney Dis. 2008;15(2):123–32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Watanabe K, Watanabe T, Nakayama M. Cerebro-renal interactions: impact of uremic toxins on cognitive function. Neurotoxicology. 2014;44:184–93.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Bugnicourt JM, Godefroy O, Chillon JM, Choukroun G, Massy ZA. Cognitive disorders and dementia in CKD: the neglected kidney-brain axis. J Am Soc Nephrol. 2013;24(3):353–63.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Mogi M, Horiuchi M. Clinical interaction between brain and kidney in small vessel disease. Cardiol Res Pract. 2011;2011:306189.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Bucur B, Madden DJ. Effects of adult age and blood pressure on executive function and speed of processing. Exp Aging Res. 2010;36(2):153–68.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Umans JG, Pliskin NH. Attention and mental processing speed in hemodialysis patients. Am J Kidney Dis. 1998;32(5):749–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Moodalbail DG, Reiser KA, Detre JA, Schultz RT, Herrington JD, Davatzikos C, et al. Systematic review of structural and functional neuroimaging findings in children and adults with CKD. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. 2013;8(8):1429–48.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Berkhout-Byrne N, Kallenberg MH, Gaasbeek A, Rabelink TJ, Hammer S, van Buchem MA, et al. The cognitive decline in older patients with end stage renal disease (COPE) study - rationale and design. Curr Med Res Opin. 2017;33(11):2057–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Levey AS, Bosch JP, Lewis JB, Greene T, Rogers N, Roth D. A more accurate method to estimate glomerular filtration rate from serum creatinine: a new prediction equation. Modification of diet in renal disease study group. Ann Intern Med. 1999;130(6):461–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. van den Brand JA, van Boekel GA, Willems HL, Kiemeney LA, den Heijer M, Wetzels JF. Introduction of the CKD-EPI equation to estimate glomerular filtration rate in a Caucasian population. Nephrol Dial Transplant. 2011;26(10):3176–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Detsky AS, McLaughlin JR, Baker JP, Johnston N, Whittaker S, Mendelson RA, et al. What is subjective global assessment of nutritional status? JPEN J Parenter Enteral Nutr. 1987;11(1):8–13.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Kruizenga HM, Seidell JC, de Vet HC, Wierdsma NJ, van Bokhorst-de van der Schueren MA. Development and validation of a hospital screening tool for malnutrition: the short nutritional assessment questionnaire (SNAQ). Clin Nutr (Edinburgh, Scotland). 2005;24(1):75–82.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Fried LP, Tangen CM, Walston J, Newman AB, Hirsch C, Gottdiener J, et al. Frailty in older adults: evidence for a phenotype. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2001;56(3):M146–56.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Kempen GI, Suurmeijer TP. The development of a hierarchical polychotomous ADL-IADL scale for noninstitutionalized elders. The Gerontologist. 1990;30(4):497–502.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. The Gerontologist. 1969;9(3):179–86.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Houx PJ, Shepherd J, Blauw GJ, Murphy MB, Ford I, Bollen EL, et al. Testing cognitive function in elderly populations: the PROSPER study. PROspective study of pravastatin in the elderly at risk. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002;73(4):385–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. van Exel E, Gussekloo J, Houx P, de Craen AJ, Macfarlane PW. Bootsma-van der Wiel a, et al. atherosclerosis and cognitive impairment are linked in the elderly. The Leiden 85-plus study. Atherosclerosis. 2002;165(2):353–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Moonen JE, Foster-Dingley JC, de Ruijter W, van der Grond J, Bertens AS, van Buchem MA, et al. Effect of discontinuation of antihypertensive treatment in elderly people on cognitive functioning--the DANTE study Leiden: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Intern Med. 2015;175(10):1622–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–98.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Adunsky A, Fleissig Y, Levenkrohn S, Arad M, Noy S. A comparative study of mini-mental test, clock drawing task and cognitive-FIM in evaluating functional outcome of elderly hip fracture patients. Clin Rehabil. 2002;16(4):414–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Suhr J, Grace J, Allen J, Nadler J, McKenna M. Quantitative and qualitative performance of stroke versus normal elderly on six clock drawing systems. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1998;13(6):495–502.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Brand N, Jolles J. Learning and retrieval rate of words presented auditorily and visually. J Gen Psychol. 1985;112(2):201–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Lindeboom J, Schmand B, Tulner L, Walstra G, Jonker C. Visual association test to detect early dementia of the Alzheimer type. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002;73(2):126–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Reitan RM. The relation of the trail making test to organic brain damage. J Consult Psychol. 1955;19(5):393–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Stroop J. Studies of interference in serial verbal reaction. J Exp Psychol. 1935;18:643–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Van der Elst W, Van Boxtel MP, Van Breukelen GJ, Jolles J. The Stroop color-word test: influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment. 2006;13(1):62–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. van Hoof JJ, Jogems-Kosterman BJ, Sabbe BG, Zitman FG, Hulstijn W. Differentiation of cognitive and motor slowing in the digit symbol test (DST): differences between depression and schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res. 1998;32(2):99–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Schmand B, Houx P, de Koning I. Published on the website of de section of Neuropsychology of the Netherlands Institute of Psychologists. 2012. Accessed via https://www.psynip.nl on April/May 2018.

  34. Wardlaw JM, Smith EE, Biessels GJ, Cordonnier C, Fazekas F, Frayne R, et al. Neuroimaging standards for research into small vessel disease and its contribution to ageing and neurodegeneration. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12(8):822–38.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Scheltens P, Barkhof F, Leys D, Pruvo JP, Nauta JJ, Vermersch P, et al. A semiquantative rating scale for the assessment of signal hyperintensities on magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurol Sci. 1993;114(1):7–12.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Jassal SV, Roscoe J, LeBlanc D, Devins GM, Rourke S. Differential impairment of psychomotor efficiency and processing speed in patients with chronic kidney disease. Int Urol Nephrol. 2008;40(3):849–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Foster R, Walker S, Brar R, Hiebert B, Komenda P, Rigatto C, et al. Cognitive impairment in advanced chronic kidney disease: the Canadian frailty observation and interventions trial. Am J Nephrol. 2016;44(6):473–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Johansen KL. The frail dialysis population: a growing burden for the dialysis community. Blood Purif. 2015;40(4):288–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Kooman JP, van der Sande FM, Leunissen KM. Kidney disease and aging: a reciprocal relation. Exp Gerontol. 2017;87(Pt B):156–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Prins ND, Scheltens P. White matter hyperintensities, cognitive impairment and dementia: an update. Nat Rev Neurol. 2015;11(3):157–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Lampe L, Kharabian-Masouleh S, Kynast J, Arelin K, Steele CJ, Loffler M, et al. Lesion location matters: the relationships between white matter hyperintensities on cognition in the healthy elderly. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2017;39(1):36.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Kloppenborg RP, Nederkoorn PJ, Geerlings MI, van den Berg E. Presence and progression of white matter hyperintensities and cognition: a meta-analysis. Neurology. 2014;82(23):2127–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Hamed SA. Neurologic conditions and disorders of uremic syndrome of chronic kidney disease: presentations, causes and treatment strategies. Expert Rev Clin Pharmacol. 2018;12(1):61.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Ikram MA, Vernooij MW, Hofman A, Niessen WJ, van der Lugt A, Breteler MM. Kidney function is related to cerebral small vessel disease. Stroke. 2008;39(1):55–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Akoudad S, Sedaghat S, Hofman A, Koudstaal PJ, van der Lugt A, Ikram MA, et al. Kidney function and cerebral small vessel disease in the general population. Int J Stroke. 2015;10(4):603–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Drew DA, Weiner DE, Tighiouart H, Scott T, Lou K, Kantor A, et al. Cognitive function and all-cause mortality in maintenance hemodialysis patients. Am J Kidney Dis. 2015;65(2):303–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Iyasere O, Okai D, Brown E. Cognitive function and advanced kidney disease: longitudinal trends and impact on decision-making. Clin Kidney J. 2017;10(1):89–94.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Broekhuizen K, Pothof A, de Craen AJ, Mooijaart SP. Characteristics of randomized controlled trials designed for elderly: a systematic review. PLoS One. 2015;10(5):e0126709.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  49. Van de Water W, Bastiaannet E, Van de Velde CJ, Liefers GJ. Inclusion and analysis of older adults in RCTs. J Gen Intern Med. 2011;26(8):831 author reply 2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

None.

Funding

The Institute for Evidence-Based Medicine in Old Age is funded by the Dutch Ministry of Health and Welfare and supported by “ZonMW” (project number 62700.3001). This study was also supported by a grand of Roche Nederland BV. The sponsor did not play a role in the preparation of this article.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Author notes

  1. Marjolijn van Buren and Simon P. Mooijaart are joint last authors of this work.

    Authors

    Contributions

    Research idea and study design: MB, SM; data acquisition: MK; NB; SH; JL; MvO; MAB; data analysis/interpretation: FD; MK; SH; MW; NB; AG; HB; MvO; JB; MAB; JL; TR; GB; MB; SM statistical analysis: FD; MK; supervision or mentorship: GB; MB; SM. Each author contributed important intellectual content during manuscript drafting or revision and accepts accountability for the overall work by ensuring that questions pertaining to the accuracy or integrity of any portion of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved. All authors have read and approved the manuscript.

    Corresponding author

    Correspondence to Floor J. van Deudekom.

    Ethics declarations

    Ethics approval and consent to participate

    The study protocol was approved by the medical ethics committee (METC) of the Leiden Univeristy Medical Centere and of all participating centres separately. From all participants written consent was obtained.

    Consent for publication

    Not applicable.

    Competing interests

    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

    Additional information

    Publisher’s Note

    Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

    Supplementary information

    Additional file 1: Supplemental Table S1.

    Performance on the different cognitive domains.

    Additional file 2: Supplemental Table S2.

    Associated characteristics of psychomotor speed.

    Additional file 3: Supplemental Table S3.

    Cerebrovascular MRI features in the study population.

    Rights and permissions

    Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

    Reprints and permissions

    About this article

    Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

    Cite this article

    van Deudekom, F.J., Kallenberg, M.H., Berkhout-Byrne, N.C. et al. Patterns and characteristics of cognitive functioning in older patients approaching end stage kidney disease, the COPE-study. BMC Nephrol 21, 126 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12882-020-01764-2

    Download citation

    • Received:

    • Accepted:

    • Published:

    • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12882-020-01764-2

    Keywords